Placeholder Image

Subtitles section Play video

  • This episode of SciShow is sponsored by Brilliant.

  • Go to Brilliant.com/SciShow to learn more.

  • {♫Intro♫}

  • There are all kinds of anecdotal cases of animals getting stoned or tipsylike,

  • the ones we talked about in our list show of high animals.

  • But as we noted for the dolphins getting high

  • on pufferfish

  • or the elephants having a nightcap of

  • marula fruit,

  • pretty much every case we've examined

  • closely has found that there isn't any evidence

  • that these animals consume mind-altering substances because they feel good to them.

  • Which ultimately makes it appear that the fondness for psychoactive drugs is, pretty...human.

  • And it raises questions about why.

  • What is it about humans that makes us so attracted to

  • these substances?

  • Totally upfront here,

  • we don't have a solid answer to that question.

  • If that's what you're looking for, you can go watch videos

  • where they say things without knowing them for sure.

  • The internet's full of them.

  • You might think we do.

  • You might have heard that these drugs

  • hijack our mental reward systems, so obviously, we seek them out because our brains have this

  • inherent weakness for them.

  • But some scientists have pointed out that that whole idea doesn't hold up as well

  • as you might think.

  • And by digging deeper into how our closest relatives and our laboratory proxies interact

  • with mind-altering substances, we can get a better understanding of what might really

  • be setting us apart.

  • The simple answer to why humans like drugs appears to be quite obvious: they feel good.

  • In neurological terms, that means that they activate the parts of our brain that signal

  • rewarding experiences.

  • And this seems pretty clear when you look at drug use in lab animals.

  • Mice and rats are used to model many human illnesses, so it makes sense that they've

  • been used to study why humans seek out drugs, too.

  • And we know that they can become addicted to almost anything: nicotine, alcohol, cocaine

  • you name it.

  • Left to their own devices, rodents will often self-administer drugs until they become very

  • intoxicated or even overdose.

  • This has allowed scientists to study the genetic basis and brain chemistry of drug-seeking

  • behaviors, as well as test ways of treating addictions.

  • And such research seems to support the idea that drugs are just really good at hijacking

  • mammalian brains.

  • But these studies don't actually confirm that non-human animals would do drugs under

  • normal circumstances, or that they are something the animals enjoy.

  • You see, the whole setup is pretty artificial.

  • In many cases, researchers use what are called self-administration models, where the animals

  • end up at least somewhat controlling how much of the drug they get.

  • But to convince lab animals to take the drugs in the first place, researchers do all kinds

  • of strange thingslike starve them.

  • That's all well and good if you want to get a rat hooked on something so you can see

  • what happens to their body or brain when they are.

  • But they aren't seeking that drug because they like itthey're motivated by a real

  • biological need like hunger.

  • Or the animals may be put in a situation where they're so bored that doing anything seems

  • better than nothing.

  • And that might be why all you have to do to disrupt their drug-seeking behavior is to

  • make their environment more ecologically realistic.

  • Lab animals often reduce their drug use or even lose their addiction when you make their

  • lives more interestinglike, if you give them friends to hang out with or place toys

  • or running wheels in their cages.

  • If these animals were truly seeking the drugs for the feeling of being high, or the drug's

  • effects were powerfully rewarding on a neurological level, it shouldn't be so easy for them

  • to just say no.

  • Of course, as we often say here on SciShow, mice and rats aren't people.

  • Neither are apes and monkeys, but they are at least primates and therefore a lot more

  • closely related to us, so they might give us more insight into why humans seek out highs.

  • And there are cases of other primates that choose to intoxicate themselveslike, on

  • the island of Saint Kitts in the Caribbean.

  • There, vervet monkeys are infamous for stealing tourists' cocktails almost right out of

  • their hands.

  • This has become a bit of a joke, but the monkeys actually inspired a series of scientific studies

  • beginning in the late 1980s.

  • One found that seventeen percent of Caribbean monkeys brought into the lab preferred a mix

  • of alcohol and sugar water over a drink of just sugar water.

  • And it seemed like the drunk monkeys were behaving pretty much like drunk peoplefalling

  • off of perches, getting cozy with other monkeys, or just sitting alone with a bottle in the corner.

  • But, we can't really tell from early experiments like this what's motivating the monkeys to drink.

  • While they eagerly consumed booze, it seems unlikely they were seeking out alcohol for

  • its intoxicating effects. Some scientists think that, instead, they may have fallen

  • into an evolutionary trap.

  • That's where a trait that was useful for the survival of a species is now harmful instead.

  • See, the taste and smell of alcohol may be similar to ripe fruit, which is more likely

  • to contain a lot of sugar and, therefore, calories.

  • And when there's lots of competition from other fruit-eaters, and relatively little

  • fruit, those monkeys want to make sure they get as many calories as they can per bite

  • to ensure they get the nutrients they need.

  • So the monkeys may have thought the presence of alcohol indicated a high-quality food.

  • In fact, when the Saint Kitts monkeys were first brought to the island around three hundred

  • years ago, they were often seen feasting on sugar cane, particularly the fermenting kind

  • that contained alcohol.

  • Seeking out that tasty, sweet treat might have later led to going for drinks that smelled

  • or tasted similar, like a tourist's fruity cocktail.

  • But, the cocktails aren't great for the monkeys nutritionally-speaking. Also, that

  • is true for the tourists. But getting drunk puts the monkeys in risk of harm in all sorts of ways.

  • So an evolved attraction to booze that was meant to help them survive may have turned

  • on them when stronger doses of alcohol became available. Then, they simply weren't able

  • to learn that the booze is harmful.

  • But, it's also important to note that only 17% of those monkeys preferred the alcoholic

  • beverage. So even if an evolutionary trap explains their preference, it's not a trap

  • all of them are falling into.

  • There are other primates that consume alcohol which we can considerlike the group of

  • chimpanzees in Guinea that regularly steals fermenting raffia palm sap.

  • But the chimps in question don't really help us figure out if primates are prone to

  • this kind of evolutionary trap, because they live in an area where people have all but

  • destroyed their habitat and natural food sources.

  • If humans weren't destroying these animal's homes and providing concentrated alcohol,

  • the animals might not go for the calorie-laden liquid.

  • In fact, in lab studies, most chimps prefer non-alcoholic fruits over fermented ones,

  • which isn't what you would expect if drinking was something universally enjoyable to them

  • or they had evolved to associate alcohol with better food sources.

  • Now, it's possible humans have fallen into an evolutionary trap of some kind when it

  • comes to psychoactive drugs.

  • But the prevailing hypothesis isn't that they were once a signal of something good,

  • and have since become bad for usit's that they trick our brains' into making

  • us think they are awesome for us, even though they aren't and never have been.

  • The basic idea is that the neurological pathways we use to evaluate things that are good for

  • us developed in the absence of psychoactive substances.

  • You see, our brains' reward system is supposed to identify things we need to survivelike

  • foodand when it activates, it can either block or overwhelm feelings that tell us something

  • is bad, like pain.

  • It's possible drugs just so happen to trigger this system even though they aren't something

  • we need to survive.

  • And if, evolutionarily speaking, they're a new thing, then we wouldn't have had time

  • to evolve countermeasures so that we don't get fooled.

  • But that idea doesn't hold up as well as you might think.

  • It's not clear when this magical time was where these substances weren't around, for example.

  • The entire lineage of humansthe homininsevolved enzymes to detoxify alcohol some ten million

  • years ago.

  • It would be pretty weird for that trait to have just evolved if we weren't regularly

  • being exposed to booze.

  • And the main group of enzymes that we use to neutralize plant toxins date back even further.

  • Cytochrome P450 haemoproteins have been detoxifying the compounds found in plants like tobacco

  • and coca for us and our other animal kin for about four hundred million years.

  • And, really, it doesn't make sense that these chemicals would accidentally affect

  • our brains so positively anyway.

  • Plants produce things like nicotine, morphine, and cocaine to be toxic and stop us and other

  • plant-eaters from consuming them.

  • Some scientists have pointed out that it doesn't really make sense for toxins meant to fend

  • off creatures like us to accidentally encourage significant consumption instead.

  • Scientists refer to this evolutionary mismatch as the paradox of drug reward.

  • One intriguing explanation for this paradox is that the triggering of reward systems isn't

  • accidental at allwe've actually evolved to find them rewarding because they have medicinal

  • value.

  • See, popular drugs like caffeine or nicotine do what the plant evolved them to do: they

  • harm animals.

  • And there are cases where we want to harm animals inside usspecifically, parasites.

  • And we see evidence for this idea in human studies. For example, a study of the Aka,

  • Indigenous people of the Congo Basin, found that men who smoked more tobacco had a lower

  • parasite burden, and they were less likely to be reinfected with a parasite one year later.

  • Many other psychoactive drugs also have antiparasitic effects.

  • If eating these plants helped us survive in the past by getting rid of nasties inside

  • us, our brains could have evolved to reward us for eating them.

  • Or, our affinity for mind-altering substances could somehow stem from our minds.

  • Basically, some researchers suggest that there are complex social or psychological benefits

  • to psychoactive substances and that those are what drive our attraction to them.

  • So, it's not that the drugshijackour brainsit's that our brains exploit

  • drugs to maximize their benefits and downplay their costs.

  • Alcohol lowers your inhibitions, for example. That's why people call itliquid courage”.

  • And if, say, you became more talkative at a party the first time you drank alcohol,

  • then even if you felt physically terrible, the reward you got from the social behavior

  • might have made the overall experience a positive one.

  • There could also be psychological benefits to drugs. Like, cocaine might give you a temporary

  • cognitive boost.

  • If the reasons we find drugs rewarding are social or psychological, then it wouldn't

  • be so surprising if they were unique to humans.

  • After all, our socialness and cognitive abilities are somewhat extreme when compared with our

  • fellow animals.

  • And we might be uniquely capable of learning how to use psychoactive substances to our

  • advantage, and not to our detriment.

  • But that's a hard hypothesis to test, in part because it's just really hard to study

  • the motivations of animals that can't tell you what they're feeling or thinking.

  • The bulk of research on animals and drugs is to understand the mechanisms of addiction.

  • Which makes sense, because it's a big public health problem.

  • And to be clear, none of these hypotheses about why we started using psychoactive substances

  • really speak to that side of drugs.

  • The neurological basis of addictionlike, that the over-use of substances which trigger

  • the reward system can lead to a chemical dependence on themholds whether that reward is an

  • accident of evolution or not.

  • Which is why models like rats and mice are still useful because if you want to study

  • the biology of addiction, it doesn't really matter how the animals get hooked, just that

  • they are.

  • But if you want to know if there are other animals that truly enjoy being high, rodents

  • can't tell you much.

  • Even studying non-human primates has ultimately left us with more questions than answers.

  • In the end, we don't really know why our

  • species is so enamored with mind-altering things.

  • But the more we look into it, the more we learn about our complex relationship with

  • psychoactive substancesand the closer we get to some real answers.

  • It's going to take a lot of research and problem-solving to get to the bottom of this.

  • And if that sounds like your cup of tea, you might want to check Brilliant's Daily Challenges.

  • Every day, Brilliant releases new challenge questions that cover fields from computer

  • science to statistics. Recently, for example, they had one about how to win at Twenty Questions.

  • Which is my kind of challenge.

  • You can access the Daily Challenges for free, but if you sign up to become a Premium member,

  • you'll get access to the whole archive. So whether you're chilling on the bus home

  • from or just want to brush up on your skills, you'll have plenty of content.

  • Also, if you really like a challenge and want to learn more about the subject, there's

  • a related course that explores the same concept in greater detail.

  • You can learn more about Brilliant.org/SciShow. And if you're one of the first two hundred

  • people to sign up there, you'll get twenty percent off an annual premium subscription.

  • {♫Outro♫}

This episode of SciShow is sponsored by Brilliant.

Subtitles and vocabulary

Click the word to look it up Click the word to find further inforamtion about it