Placeholder Image

Subtitles section Play video

  • How do you define pain? You could ask a thousand people and probably get a thousand different

  • answersIt's both physiological and psychological. It's emotional and culturalAccording

  • to the Association for the Study of Pain in 1979, pain isan unpleasant sensory and

  • emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in

  • terms of tissue damage.” But how did we arrive at this maybe-not-so-crystal-clear

  • definition? And what does pain theory teach us about our own bodies?

  • This question has been the subject of scientific and philosophical debate since Ancient Greece, but in the last

  • few centuries, it hinged on one question: where does pain come from physiologically?

  • Is there a specific set of nerves for pain? And if not, how do our regular nerves

  • know when something is painful or not painful? And is this even happening at the nerve level,

  • or is the brain more involved? Well, in 1811 a surgeon named Charles Bell attempted to

  • answer that question. His idea was that specific nerve fibers send pain signals back to the

  • brain, just like how any other sensory fibers like vision or smell send signals to the brain.

  • We call this specificity theory today. And experiments by physiologists in the years

  • afterwards seemed to back him up. Scientists identified specific receptors for all kinds

  • of different sensationsincluding loads of different touch receptors embedded in your

  • skin. Like Meissner's corpuscles are stimulated when something textured moves over our fingertips.

  • And those are different than Pacinian corpuscles which only sense signals for fine textures

  • and high frequency vibration. Or how Merkel's discs help us determine things like pressure

  • while Ruffini corpuscles are probably responsible for sensing stretching in your skin. These

  • four types of receptors all sense mechanical touch, so we call them mechanoreceptorsLikewise,

  • the receptors for pain were named nociceptorsnoci- for noxious, or painful. Then in

  • the early twentieth century, the main competing theory was pattern theory,

  • and it was pretty much the opposite. According to pattern theory, our nerves send signals

  • to the brain, and if the brain detects a familiar pattern of pain stimuli, the signals will

  • be interpreted as pain. As anatomists kept discovering new sensory receptors, pattern

  • theory fell out of favor and specificity theory was more accepted. But specificity theory

  • still had some issues. The most important was that pain isn't just the result of physical

  • injury. It's a complex experience involving emotional, psychological, and cognitive factors.

  • So in 1965, two scientists published a paper describing what they called the gate control

  • theory, which is the most popular model of pain physiology used today. This theory says

  • that signals from the peripheral nerves first have to travel to three areas of the spinal

  • cord before traveling to the brainthe substantia gelatinosa, the fibers of the dorsal

  • column of the spinal cord, and specialized cells called transmission cells in the dorsal

  • horn as well. Those nerves from the mechanoreceptors and nociceptors all converge at this gate,

  • the substantia gelatinosa. If a signal from a nociceptor reaches a certain intensity,

  • the gate will open and that signal will be sent to the brain where it's interpreted

  • as pain. But if the gate is closed, the signal never makes it farther than the spinal cord.

  • So it wasn't a straight route from skin to brain after all, pain had to pass through

  • a gate first. Those researchers in 1965 suggested a few ways that the central nervous system

  • could close that gate, including moderation from the brain itself. This was an important

  • step in pain theory that demonstrated how the brain and peripheral nerves both influenced

  • how we feel painThese days, we've added a few more details that give us a more complete

  • model of pain perception. We now know that not all nociceptors signal for the same types

  • of painWe have receptors for high intensity mechanical pain, like getting punched, but

  • different ones for high temperature, like stepping on a George Foreman grill.

  • "I clamped down on my foot. That's it. I don't see what's so hard to believe about that."

  • And these nociceptors aren't just in your skin. They're found in joints, muscles, and around organs.

  • Most of those receptors are attached to one of two major nerve types, either a Type A-delta

  • fiber or a Type C fiberMore of those Type A Delta fibers are wider and covered in a

  • substance called myelin that lets them send signals faster than the unmyelinated Type

  • C fibers. And this lends itself to a cool physiology hackType A Delta fibers send

  • signals to the spinal cord faster, they're responsible for the first perception of pain,

  • while the slow Type C fibers convey pain intensity. But the mechanoreceptors are myelinated too,

  • so if you stimulate any of the non-painful touch receptors, that signal arrives at the

  • substantia gelatinosa first, closing the gate. That's why rubbing a banged up elbow works.

  • You're taking advantage of the gate control theory. Then again it might all be in vain.

  • Life is pain, and anyone who says otherwise is selling something. Did I write this entire

  • episode so I could sneak in a Princess Bride quote? You bet I did! Thanks for watching

  • this episode of Seeker Human. We've got more episodes like this one coming, so make

  • sure you're subscribed to us on YouTube and following us on social media so you don't miss a single video.

How do you define pain? You could ask a thousand people and probably get a thousand different

Subtitles and vocabulary

Click the word to look it up Click the word to find further inforamtion about it