Subtitles section Play video
Why do smart people make dumb decisions?
Why do conspiracy theorists think that we didn’t land on the moon or that Hillary
Clinton is a space alien?
And why won’t Bernice admit that the new Superman movie just isn’t very good?
We’ve talked about cognition before. We usually refer to it as the process that we
use to think and solve crossword puzzles and stuff. But really, cognition involves knowing,
remembering, understanding, communicating, and to a certain extent, learning.
And as truly wonderful as our brains are, we can be spectacularly bad at ALL of these
things.
We used to think our cognition worked like a computer -- logically processing information.
But that cabbage-sized chunk of pink, wet brain matter in your skull can do a lot more
than math, and the things that it does are certainly not always logical.
Many experts argue that it’s cognition that makes us truly human, and that everything
that comes with it -- our preferences, prejudices, fears, and intuitions -- are what make us
the individuals that we are.
We’re not the only animals that show some evidence of cognition, of course: Chimps and
gorillas exhibit insight and planning; crows use tools; elephants teach each other.
But our capacity as humans to figure stuff out is matched only by our ability to totally
misjudge stuff. As prone as we are to brilliance and insight, we’re equally likely to succumb
to irrational thinking and false intuition.
So, to borrow a riff from Rene Descartes, you think, therefore you are.
Which means you’re brilliant a lot of the time. And sometimes, you’re just going to
look stupid.
[INTRO]
We all want to make sense of the world. And one of the major ways our cognition allows
us do that is by forming concepts -- mental groupings of similar objects, people, ideas,
or events. We like to lump things together.
Concepts simplify our thinking in such a fundamental way that we usually don’t have to stop and
think about using them, they’re just there.
And yet without concepts, we’d need a unique name for everything. You couldn’t just ask
me to shake the anglerfish -- because there’d be no concept of shake or fish, let alone
stuffed, blue anglerfish.
And if I told you I was devastated that I lost my anglerfish -- which I probably would
be -- I’d also have to explain my emotions, their intensities, even the words themselves
that I had to use.
So basically, without concepts, no one would ever get anything done. We’d all be like
a bunch of ents taking all morning to say “Hey, what’s up?”
We often organize our concepts by forming prototypes--mental images or pinnacle examples
of a certain thing.
For example, if I say “bird”--the general shape of a songbird probably pops into your
head before like, a penguin or chicken or emu, because robins and cardinals more closely
resemble our bird prototype. Still, if I show you a picture of some crazy
creature you’ve never seen before, and you note that it has feathers and a beak, you’ll
probably file it under the bird category because it more closely resembles your concept of
bird than your concept of rodent or overcoat or footstool.
Concepts and prototypes speed up our thinking, but they also can box in our thinking, and
lead to prejudice if we see something that doesn’t fit our prototypes.
A hundred years ago the sight of a female doctor might have caused some heads to explode,
because in peoples’ tiny minds, the prototypes of “doctor” and “woman” didn’t have
any overlap. And actually some people today still feel that way. Haters gonna hate.
So it’s important to actively keep your mind open mind to make room for evolving concepts,
and remember that concepts may sometimes hurt as much as they help.
One of the biggest ways our cognition works to our benefit, though, is through our ability
to solve problems. We use our problem-solving skills all the
time: How to assemble Scandinavian furniture, bake muffins with a missing ingredient, or
handle the crushing disappointment of the new Superman movie.
And we approach problem-solving in different ways -- sometimes we value speed; other times,
accuracy.
Some problems we figure out using trial and error--you know, you try something and if
it doesn’t work, try it a different way, and keep at it until something works. Trial
and error is slow and deliberate--which may be good or bad, depending on the problem.
We can also use algorithms and heuristics to come up with solutions.
Algorithms are logical, methodical, step-by-step procedures that guarantee an eventual solution,
though they may be slow to work through. Heuristics, on the other hand, are more like
mental shortcuts -- simple strategies that allow us to solve problems faster, although
they’re more error-prone than algorithms. Say you’re at the store, looking for a family-sized
bottle of Sriracha. You could use an algorithm and methodically check every shelf and aisle
in the store. Or you could use heuristics and first search the Asian or condiment sections--the
places that make the most sense based on what you already know.
Heuristics may be way faster, but the algorithmic approach guarantees you won’t overlook the
sauce along the way, because they stuck it in the deli or whatever dumb thing they did
this week.
So algorithms, heuristics, and trial-and-error are problem-solving strategies that involve
a plan of attack. But sometimes we get lucky while puzzling
out a problem, and Aha!, out of nowhere a sudden flash of insight that solves our problem.
I’ll use orange in the muffin recipe instead of lemon! Or, Sriracha lives in the Mexican
section! For some reason! Neuroscientists have actually watched that
kind of sudden, happy brain flash on neuroimaging screens.
In one experiment, they gave subjects a problem to solve:
What word can be added to the three words CRAB, PINE, and SAUCE to create a new compound
word? Then they asked the subjects to press a button
when they had the answer.
While the subjects thought about it, scans showed activity in their frontal lobes, the
areas involved in the focused attention of typical problem-solving.
But right at the Aha! moment, just as they pushed the button, there was a clear burst
of activity just above the ear in the right temporal lobe, which, among many other things,
is involved with recognition.
The answer, by the way, we already gave you the hint earlier in the episode.
Where’s my fish? Those sudden bursts of insight are awesome,
but you can’t count on them to solve all your problems. And just because something
feels, doesn’t mean it’s truly correct. Because as inventive and smartypants as we
may be, our cognition often leads us astray in all kinds of ways.
For instance, we often look for, and favor, evidence that verifies our ideas, while we’re
more likely to avoid or ignore contradictory evidence -- a tendency known as confirmation
bias. This is really similar to the overconfidence we’ve talked about, when you’re basically
more confident than you are correct. When this kind of cognitive bias takes hold,
you might cling to your initial conceptions in a kind of belief perseverance, even in
the face of clear proof to the contrary. This happens all the time, and it can be maddening
for people watching it happen. People still think that the earth is flat! It’s like...WHAT?
HOW? There’s space pictures! I probably don’t need to tell you -- people
can really get weird and defensive when they evade facts and choose to see only the information
that confirms their beliefs. They may even become functionally fixed, unable
to view a problem from a new perspective. Instead they just keep approaching a situation
with the same mental set, especially if it’s worked in the past.
Say you’ve got a nail sticking out from a board, and you’re like “I need to take
care of that!” There’s rocks, and bricks all around you. But because of your functional
fixedness on the idea that only hammers work on nails, you don’t even consider hitting
it with the brick, and instead you waste a bunch of time in the garage looking for a
hammer, and you’re angry and frustrated, and there’s still a nail sticking up from
the board. So, our mental set predisposes how we think,
just as you’ll remember that our perceptual set predisposes how we perceive.
This is what makes heuristics -- those super-convenient mental shortcuts that we all use -- so easily
fallible. In the 1970s, cognitive psychologists Amos
Tversky and Daniel Kahneman researched how we make snap judgments, and discovered one
way smart people make dumb decisions. They found that people believe an event will
be more likely to occur if they can conjure up examples or memories of it, especially
if those examples are particularly vivid, scary, or awesome.
So, say you’re in a casino and you win two dollars at a slot machine. Suddenly every
flashing light and ringing bell in the place goes off. But when you lose -- which is the
vast majority of the time -- it’s just...crickets. With all their lights and noise-making, the
casino makes sure that wins are super vivid and memorable, while losses just go away unacknowledged.
That way, the next time you’re standing there with 100 bucks in your pocket, you’re
more likely to overestimate your chances of winning, because the memories of winning are
more striking. The more mentally available those memories
are, the more it seems that it’s going to happen again. This is known as the availability
heuristic. And it can warp our judgements of people,
too. If we keep remembering news footage that shows people of a given group shooting guns,
that can shape our impression of the entire group -- even if what we saw was only a tiny
minority within that group. Essentially, we are great at fearing the wrong
things. We worry about being killed in a plane crash or getting bitten in half by a shark
or accidentally choking on a dumpling. Thanks to our brain’s b-roll of horrific
images, we come to fear what’s actually very rare, instead of worrying about much
more common, but less memorable ends like car accidents, cancer, and heart failure.
Our thinking can also be swayed by framing, or how an issue is presented. Imagine you’re
considering climbing Everest or getting a nose job or eating a bowl of raw blowfish.
I can frame the risks in different ways. Telling you that you’ve got a 95 percent chance
of survival sounds a lot different than saying five out of a hundred people die doing this
activity, though the information is the same. Our cognitive minds are capable of incredible
intellectual feats and tremendous failures. We can solve problems better than any organism
on the planet, but given the chance, we can also mess up a pretty simple judgment every
day of the week. But if we’re mindful of our capacity for
error -- and if we honor our ingenuity and intellect -- I think our ability to solve
any problem is nearly infinite. And that, gives me a lot of hope.
Seriously though where is my fish? Today you learned how we use concepts, prototypes,
and our mental sets to think and communicate, and how algorithms, heuristics, and insight
help us solve problems. You also learned about how fixation, the availability heuristic,
fear, overconfidence, and belief perseverance can get in the way of good decision-making
and thinking. Thank you for watching, especially to our
Subbable subscribers, who make this whole channel possible. If you’d like to sponsor
an episode of Crash Course, get a special Laptop Decal, or even be animated into an
upcoming episode, just go to Subbable.com/crashcourse. This episode was written by Kathleen Yale,
edited by Blake de Pastino, and our consultant is Dr. Ranjit Bhagwat. Our director and editor
is Nicholas Jenkins, the script supervisor is Michael Aranda, who is also our sound designer,
and the graphics team is Thought Café.